As there are one and half billion cattle romping the earth. The predominance of cattle to western culture never has been
more evident.
Nearly all most every food source has dairy. For example behaviour as
our identity mixed with animal. On another world this would be alien and a
culture. As cattle are a pray they Haven two kinds of colour receptors in
the cone cells of their retinas. This means that cattle are dichromatic, as are
most other non-primate land mammals. There are two to three rods per cone in
the fovea centralis but five to six near the optic papilla. Cattle can
distinguish long wavelength colours (yellow, orange and red) much better than
the shorter wavelengths (blue, grey and green). Calves are able to discriminate
between long (red) and short (blue) or medium (green) wavelengths, but have
limited ability to discriminate between the short and medium.
They also
approach handlers more quickly under red light. Whilst having good colour
sensitivity, it is not as good as humans or sheep. A common misconception about
cattle (particularly bulls) is that they are enraged by the colour red
(something provocative is often said to be "like a red flag to a
bull"). This is a myth. In bullfighting, it is the movement of the red
flag or cape that irritates the bull and incites it to charge Cattle eat mixed
diets, but when given the opportunity, show a partial preference of
approximately 70% clover and 30% grass.
This preference has a diurnal pattern,
with a stronger preference for clover in the morning, and the proportion of
grass increasing towards the evening. In laboratory studies, young cattle are
able to memorize the locations of several food sources and retain this memory
for at least 8 hrs, although this declined after 12 hrs. Fifteen-month-old heifers learn more quickly than adult cows which have had
either one or two calving, but their longer-term memory is less stable. Mature
cattle perform well in spatial learning tasks and have a good long-term memory
in these tests. Cattle tested in a radial arm maze are able to remember the
locations of high-quality food for at least 30 days. Although they initially
learn to avoid low-quality food, this memory diminishes over the same duration.



Under less artificial testing conditions, young cattle
showed they were able to remember the location of feed for at least 48 days.
Cattle
can make an association between a visual stimulus and food within 1 day –
memory of this association can be retained for 1 year, despite a slight decay.
Calves are capable of discrimination learning [50] and adult cattle compare
favourably with small mammals in their learning ability in the Closed-field
Test. They are also able to discriminate between familiar individuals, and
among humans. Cattle can tell the difference between familiar and unfamiliar
animals of the same species (conspecifics). Studies show they behave less
aggressively toward familiar individuals when they are forming a new
group.Calves can also discriminate between humans based on previous experience,
as shown by approaching those who handled them positively and avoiding those
who handled them aversely. Although cattle can discriminate between humans by
their faces alone, they also use other cues such as the colour of clothes when
these are available.
In audio play-back studies, calves prefer their own mother's vocalizations compared to the vocalizations of an unfamiliar mother. In laboratory studies using images, cattle can discriminate between images of the heads of cattle and other animal species. [56] They are also able to distinguish between familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics. Furthermore, they are able to categorize images as familiar and unfamiliar individuals. When mixed with other individuals, cloned calves from the same donor form subgroups, indicating that kin discrimination occurs and may be a basis of grouping behaviour. It has also been shown using images of cattle that both artificially inseminated and cloned calves have similar cognitive capacities of kin and non-kin discrimination.

In audio play-back studies, calves prefer their own mother's vocalizations compared to the vocalizations of an unfamiliar mother. In laboratory studies using images, cattle can discriminate between images of the heads of cattle and other animal species. [56] They are also able to distinguish between familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics. Furthermore, they are able to categorize images as familiar and unfamiliar individuals. When mixed with other individuals, cloned calves from the same donor form subgroups, indicating that kin discrimination occurs and may be a basis of grouping behaviour. It has also been shown using images of cattle that both artificially inseminated and cloned calves have similar cognitive capacities of kin and non-kin discrimination.
Cattle can recognize familiar individuals. Visual individual
recognition is distinguished from mere visual discrimination.
Recognition is a
more complex mental process than discrimination. It requires the recollection
of the learned idiosyncratic identity of an individual that has been previously
encountered and the formation of a mental representation. By using
2-dimensional images of the heads of one cow (face, profiles, ¾ views), all the
tested heifers showed individual recognition of familiar and unfamiliar
individuals from their own breed. Furthermore, almost all the heifers
recognized unknown individuals from different breeds, although this was
achieved with greater difficulty. Individual recognition was most difficult
when the visual features of the breed being tested were quite different from
the breed in the image, for example, the breed being tested had no spots
whereas the image was of a spotted breed.
Cattle use visual/brain
lateralisation in their visual scanning of novel and familiar stimuli. Domestic
cattle prefer to view novel stimuli with the left eye, i.e. using the right
brain hemisphere (similar to horses, Australian magpies, chicks, toads and
fish) but use the right eye, i.e. using the left hemisphere, for viewing
familiar stimuli. In cattle, temperament can affect production traits such as
carcass and meat quality or milk yield as well as affecting the animal's
overall health and reproduction. Cattle temperament is defined as "the
consistent behavioural and physiological difference observed between
individuals in response to a stressor or environmental challenge and is used to
describe the relatively stable difference in the behavioural predisposition of
an animal, which can be related to psychobiological mechanisms".
Generally, cattle temperament is assumed to be multidimensional. Five
underlying categories of temperament traits have been proposed. There demeanour
shyness-boldness, exploration-avoidance, activity, aggressiveness and sociability.


In a study on Holstein–Friesian heifers learning to press a
panel to open a gate for access to a food reward.
The researchers also recorded
the heart rate and behavior of the heifers when moving along the race towards
the food. When the heifers made clear improvements in learning, they had higher
heart rates and tended to move more vigorously along the race. The researchers
concluded this was an indication that cattle may react emotionally to their own
learning improvement. Negative emotional states are associated with a bias
toward negative (pessimistic) responses towards ambiguous cues in judgement
tasks –as encapsulated in the question of "is the glass half empty or half
full?".
After separation from their mothers. Holstein calves showed such a
cognitive bias indicative of low mood. A similar study showed that after
hot-iron dis-budding (dehorning), calves had a similar negative bias indicating
that post-operative pain following this routine procedure results in a negative
change in emotional state. In studies of visual discrimination, the position of
the ears has been used as an indicator of emotional state.When cattle are
stressed, this can be recognised by other cattle as it is communicated by alarm
substances in the urine. Cattle are very gregarious and even short-term
isolation is considered to cause severe psychological stress.
When Aubrac and
Fresian heifers are isolated, they increase their vocalizations and experience
increased heart rate and plasma cortisol concentrations. These physiological
changes are greater in Aubracs. When visual contact is re-instated,
vocalisations rapidly decline, regardless of the familiarity of the returning
cattle, however, heart rate decreases are greater if the returning cattle are
familiar to the previously-isolated individual. Mirrors have been used to
reduce stress in isolated cattle. There is conflicting evidence for
magnetoreception in cattle. One study reported that resting and grazing cattle
tend to align their body axes in the geomagnetic North-South (N-S) direction
this wouldn't be the case.


